What’s the Trouble?
How doctors think.
By Jerome Groopman
January 22, 2007
n a spring afternoon several years ago, Evan McKinley was hiking in the woods near Halifax, Nova Scotia, when he felt a
sharp pain in his chest. McKinley (a pseudonym) was a forest ranger in his early forties, trim and extremely t. He had felt
discomfort in his chest for several days, but this was more severe: it hurt each time he took a breath. McKinley slowly made his
way through the woods to a shed that housed his office, where he sat and waited for the pain to pass. He frequently carried heavy
packs on his back and was used to muscle aches, but this pain felt different. He decided to see a doctor.
Pat Croskerry was the physician in charge in the emergency room at Dartmouth General Hospital, near Halifax, that day. He
listened intently as McKinley described his symptoms. He noted that McKinley was a muscular man; that his face was ruddy, as
would be expected of someone who spent most of his day outdoors; and that he was not sweating. (Perspiration can be a sign of
cardiac distress.) McKinley told him that the pain was in the center of his chest, and that it had not spread into his arms, neck, or
back. He told Croskerry that he had never smoked or been overweight; had no family history of heart attack, stroke, or diabetes;
and was under no particular stress. His family life was ne, McKinley said, and he loved his job.
Croskerry checked McKinley’s blood pressure, which was normal, and his pulse, which was sixty and regular—typical for an
athletic man. Croskerry listened to McKinley’s lungs and heart, but detected no abnormalities. When he pressed on the spot
between McKinley’s ribs and breastbone, McKinley felt no pain. There was no swelling or tenderness in his calves or thighs.
Finally, the doctor ordered an electrocardiogram, a chest X-ray, and blood tests to measure McKinley’s cardiac enzymes.
(Abnormal levels of cardiac enzymes indicate damage to the heart.) As Croskerry expected, the results of all the tests were normal.
Most physicians already have in mind two or three possible diagnoses within minutes of meeting a patient. Photograph by Guy Billout
8/6/2020 How Doctors Think | The New Yorker
https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2007/01/29/whats-the-trouble?utm_source=nl&utm_brand=tny&utm_mailing=TNY_Classics_Daily_080520… 2/7
C
“I’m not at all worried about your chest pain,” Croskerry told McKinley, before sending him home. “You probably overexerted
yourself in the eld and strained a muscle. My suspicion that this is coming from your heart is about zero.”
Early the next evening, when Croskerry arrived at the emergency room to begin his shift, a colleague greeted him. “Very
interesting case, that man you saw yesterday,” the doctor said. “He came in this morning with an acute myocardial infarction.”
Croskerry was shocked. The colleague tried to console him. “If I had seen this guy, I wouldn’t have gone as far as you did in
ordering all those tests,” he said. But Croskerry knew that he had made an error that could have cost the ranger his life. (McKinley
survived.) “Clearly, I missed it,” Croskerry told me, referring to McKinley’s heart attack. “And why did I miss it? I didn’t miss it
because of any egregious behavior, or negligence. I missed it because my thinking was overly inuenced by how healthy this man
looked, and the absence of risk factors.”
roskerry, who is sixty-four years old, began his career as an experimental psychologist, studying rats’ brains in the laboratory.
In 1979, he decided to become a doctor, and, as a medical student, he was surprised at how little attention was paid to what
he calls the “cognitive dimension” of clinical decision-making—the process by which doctors interpret their patients’ symptoms
and weigh test results in order to arrive at a diagnosis and a plan of treatment. Students spent the rst two years of medical school
memorizing facts about physiology, pharmacology, and pathology; they spent the last two learning practical applications for this
knowledge, such as how to decipher an EKG and how to determine the appropriate dose of insulin for a diabetic. Croskerry’s
instructors rarely bothered to describe the mental logic they relied on to make a correct diagnosis and avoid mistakes.
In 1990, Croskerry became the head of the emergency department at Dartmouth General Hospital, and was struck by the number
of errors made by doctors under his supervision. He kept lists of the errors, trying to group them into categories, and, in the midnineties, he began to publish articles in medical journals, borrowing insights from cognitive psychology to explain how doctors
make clinical decisions—especially awed ones—under the stressful conditions of the emergency room. “Emergency physicians are
required to make an unusually high number of decisions in the course of their work,” he wrote in “Achieving Quality in Clinical
Decision Making: Cognitive Strategies and Detection of Bias,” an article published in Academic Emergency Medicine, in 2002.
These doctors’ decisions necessarily entail a great deal of uncertainty, Croskerry wrote, since, “for the most part, patients are not
known and their illnesses are seen through only small windows of focus and time.” By calling physicians’ attention to common
mistakes in medical judgment, he has helped to promote an emerging eld in medicine: the study of how doctors think.
There are limited data about the frequency of misdiagnoses. Research from the nineteen-eighties and nineties suggests that they
occur in about fteen per cent of cases, but Croskerry suspects that the rate is signicantly higher. He believes that many
misdiagnoses are the result of readily identiable—and often preventable—errors in thinking.
Doctors typically begin to diagnose patients the moment they meet them. Even before they conduct an examination, they are
interpreting a patient’s appearance: his complexion, the tilt of his head, the movements of his eyes and mouth, the way he sits or
stands up, the sound of his breathing. Doctors’ theories about what is wrong continue to evolve as they listen to the patient’s heart,
or press on his liver. But research shows that most physicians already have in mind two or three possible diagnoses within minutes
of meeting a patient, and that they tend to develop their hunches from very incomplete information. To make diagnoses, most
doctors rely on shortcuts and rules of thumb—known in psychology as “heuristics.”
Heuristics are indispensable in medicine; physicians, particularly in emergency rooms, must often make quick judgments about
how to treat a patient, on the basis of a few, potentially serious symptoms. A doctor is trained to assume, for example, that a
patient suffering from a high fever and sharp pain in the lower right side of the abdomen could be suffering from appendicitis; he
immediately sends the patient for X-rays and contacts the surgeon on call. But, just as heuristics can help doctors save lives, they
can also lead them to make grave errors. In retrospect, Croskerry realized that when he saw McKinley in the emergency room the
ranger had been experiencing unstable angina—a surge of chest pain that is caused by coronary-artery disease and that may
8/6/2020 How Doctors Think | The New Yorker
https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2007/01/29/whats-the-trouble?utm_source=nl&utm_brand=tny&utm_mailing=TNY_Classics_Daily_080520… 3/7
D
precede a heart attack. “The unstable angina didn’t show on the EKG, because fty per cent of such cases don’t,” Croskerry said.
“His unstable angina didn’t show up on the cardiac-enzymes test, because there had been no damage to his heart muscle yet. And
it didn’t show up on the chest X-ray, because the heart had not yet begun to fail, so there was no uid backed up in the lungs.”
The mistake that Croskerry made is called a “representativeness” error. Doctors make such errors when their thinking is overly
inuenced by what is typically true; they fail to consider possibilities that contradict their mental templates of a disease, and thus
attribute symptoms to the wrong cause. Croskerry told me that he had immediately noticed the ranger’s trim frame: most t men
in their forties are unlikely to be suffering from heart disease. Moreover, McKinley’s pain was not typical of coronary-artery
disease, and the results of the physical examination and the blood tests did not suggest a heart problem. But, Croskerry
emphasized, this was precisely the point: “You have to be prepared in your mind for the atypical and not be too quick to reassure
yourself, and your patient, that everything is O.K.” (Croskerry could have kept McKinley under observation and done a second
cardiac-enzyme test or had him take a cardiac stress test, which might have revealed the source of his chest pain.) When Croskerry
teaches students and interns about representativeness errors, he cites Evan McKinley as an example.
octors can also make mistakes when their judgments about a patient are unconsciously inuenced by the symptoms and
illnesses of patients they have just seen. Many common infections tend to occur in epidemics, afflicting large numbers of
people in a single community at the same time; after a doctor sees six patients with, say, the u, it is common to assume that the
seventh patient who complains of similar symptoms is suffering from the same disease. Harrison Alter, an emergency-room
physician, recently confronted this problem. At the time, Alter was working in the emergency room of a hospital in Tuba City,
Arizona, which is situated on a Navajo reservation. In a three-week period, dozens of people had come to his hospital suffering
from viral pneumonia. One day, Blanche Begaye (a pseudonym), a Navajo woman in her sixties, arrived at the emergency room
complaining that she was having trouble breathing. Begaye was a compact woman with long gray hair that she wore in a bun. She
told Alter that she had begun to feel unwell a few days earlier. At rst, she said, she had thought that she had a bad head cold, so
she had drunk orange juice and tea, and taken a few aspirin. But her symptoms had got worse. Alter noted that she had a fever of
100.2 degrees, and that she was breathing rapidly—at almost twice the normal rate. He listened to her lungs but heard none of the
harsh sounds, called rhonchi, that suggest an accumulation of mucus. A chest X-ray showed that Begaye’s lungs did not have the
white streaks typical of viral pneumonia, and her white-blood-cell count was not elevated, as would be expected if she had the
illness.
However, a blood test to measure her electrolytes revealed that her blood had become slightly acidic, which can occur in the case of
a major infection. Alter told Begaye that he thought she had “subclinical pneumonia.” She was in the early stages of the infection,
he said; the virus had not yet affected her lungs in a way that would show up on a chest X-ray. He ordered her to be admitted to
the hospital and given intravenous uids and medicine to bring her fever down. Viral pneumonia can tax an older person’s heart
and sometimes cause it to fail, he told her, so it was prudent that she remain under observation by doctors. Alter referred Begaye to
the care of an internist on duty and began to examine another patient.
A few minutes later, the internist approached Alter and took him aside. “That’s not a case of viral pneumonia,” the doctor said.
“She has aspirin toxicity.”
Immediately, Alter knew that the internist was right. Aspirin toxicity occurs when patients overdose on the drug, causing
hyperventilation and the accumulation of lactic acid and other acids in the blood. “Aspirin poisoning—bread-and-butter
toxicology,” Alter told me. “This was something that was drilled into me throughout my training. She was an absolutely classic
case—the rapid breathing, the shift in her blood electrolytes—and I missed it. I got cavalier.”
Alter’s misdiagnosis resulted from the use of a heuristic called “availability,” which refers to the tendency to judge the likelihood of
an event by the ease with which relevant examples come to mind. This tendency was rst described in 1973, in a paper by Amos
8/6/2020 How Doctors Think | The New Yorker
https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2007/01/29/whats-the-trouble?utm_source=nl&utm_brand=tny&utm_mailing=TNY_Classics_Daily_080520… 4/7
R
Tversky and Daniel Kahneman, psychologists at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem. For example, a businessman may estimate
the likelihood that a given venture could fail by recalling difficulties that his associates had encountered in the marketplace, rather
than by relying on all the data available to him about the venture; the experiences most familiar to him can bias his assessment of
the chances for success. (Kahneman won the Nobel Prize in Economics in 2002, for his research on decision-making under
conditions of uncertainty.) The diagnosis of subclinical pneumonia was readily available to Alter, because he had recently seen so
many cases of the infection. Rather than try to integrate all the information he had about Begaye’s illness, he had focussed on the
symptoms that she shared with other patients he had seen: her fever, her rapid breathing, and the acidity of her blood. He
dismissed the data that contradicted his diagnosis—the absence of rhonchi and of white streaks on the chest X-ray, and the normal
white-blood-cell count—as evidence that the infection was at an early stage. In fact, this information should have made him doubt
his hypothesis. (Psychologists call this kind of cognitive cherry-picking “conrmation bias”: conrming what you expect to nd by
selectively accepting or ignoring information.)
After the internist made the correct diagnosis, Alter recalled his conversation with Begaye. When he had asked whether she had
taken any medication, including over-the-counter drugs, she had replied, “A few aspirin.” As Alter told me, “I didn’t dene with
her what ‘a few’ meant.” It turned out to be several dozen.
epresentativeness and availability errors are intellectual mistakes, but the errors that doctors make because of their feelings
for a patient can be just as signicant. We all want to believe that our physician likes us and is moved by our plight. Doctors,
in turn, are encouraged to develop positive feelings for their patients; caring is generally held to be the cornerstone of humanistic
medicine. Sometimes, however, a doctor’s impulse to protect a patient he likes or admires can adversely affect his judgment.
In 1979, I treated Brad Miller (a pseudonym), a young literature instructor who was suffering from bone cancer. I was living in
Los Angeles at the time, completing a fellowship in hematology and oncology at the U.C.L.A. Medical Center. “You look
familiar,” Brad said to me when I introduced myself to him in his hospital room as the doctor who would be overseeing his care. “I
see you running with two or three friends around the university,” he said. “I’m a runner, too—or, at least, was.”
I told Brad that I hoped he would be able to run again soon, though I warned him that his chemotherapy treatment would be
difficult.
About six weeks earlier, Brad had noticed an ache in his left knee. He had been training to run in a marathon, and at rst he
thought that the ache was caused by a sore muscle. He saw a specialist in sports medicine, who examined the leg and
recommended that he wear a knee brace when he ran. Brad followed this advice, but the ache got worse. The physician ordered an
X-ray, which showed an osteosarcoma, a cancerous growth, around the end of the femur, just above the knee.
Several years earlier, the surgical-oncology department at U.C.L.A. had devised an experimental treatment for this kind of
sarcoma, involving a new chemotherapy drug called Adriamycin. Oncologists had nicknamed Adriamycin “the red death,” because
of its cranberry color and its toxicity. Not only did it cause severe nausea, vomiting, mouth blisters, and reduced blood counts;
repeated doses could injure cardiac muscle and lead to heart failure. Patients had to be monitored closely, since once the heart is
damaged there is no good way to restore its pumping capacity. Still, doctors at U.C.L.A. had found that giving patients multiple
doses of Adriamycin often shrank tumors, allowing them to surgically remove the cancer without amputating the affected limb—
the standard approach in the past.
I began administering the treatment that afternoon. Despite taking Compazine to stave off vomiting, Brad was acutely nauseated.
After several doses of chemotherapy, his white-blood-cell count dropped precipitately. Because his immune system was weakened,
he was at great risk of contracting an infection. I required visitors to Brad’s room to wear a mask, a gown, and gloves, and
instructed the nurses not to give him raw food, in order to limit his exposure to bacteria.
8/6/2020 How Doctors Think | The New Yorker
https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2007/01/29/whats-the-trouble?utm_source=nl&utm_brand=tny&utm_mailing=TNY_Classics_Daily_080520… 5/7
B
L
“Not to your taste,” I said at the end of the rst week of treatment, seeing an untouched meal on his tray.
“My mouth hurts,” Brad whispered. “And, even if I could chew, it looks pretty tasteless.”
I agreed that the food looked dismal.
“What is to your taste?” I asked. “Fried kidney?”
I had told Brad when we met that I had studied “Ulysses” in college, in a freshman seminar. The professor had explained the
relevant Irish history, the subtle references to Catholic liturgy, and a number of other allusions that most of us in the class would
otherwise not have grasped. I had enjoyed Joyce’s descriptions of Leopold Bloom eating fried kidneys.
rad was my favorite patient on the ward. Each morning when I made rounds with the residents and the medical students, I
would take an inventory of his symptoms and review his laboratory results. I would often linger a few moments in his room,
trying to distract him from the misery of his therapy by talking about literature.
The treatment called for a scan after the third cycle of Adriamycin. If the cancer had shrunk sufficiently, the surgery would
proceed. If it hadn’t, or if the cancer had grown despite the chemotherapy, then there was little to be done short of amputation.
Even after amputation, patients with osteosarcomas are at risk of a recurrence.
One morning, Brad developed a low-grade fever. During rounds, the residents told me that they had taken blood and urine
cultures and that Brad’s physical examination was “nonfocal”—they had found no obvious reason for the fever. Patients often get
low fevers during chemotherapy after their white-blood-cell count falls; if the fever has no identiable cause, the doctor must
decide whether and when to administer a course of antibiotics.
“So you feel even more wiped out?” I asked Brad.
He nodded. I asked him about various symptoms that could help me determine what was causing the fever. Did he have a
headache? Difficulty seeing? Pressure in his sinuses? A sore throat? Problems breathing? Pain in his abdomen? Diarrhea? Burning
on urination? He shook his head.
Two residents helped prop Brad up in bed so that I could examine him; I had a routine that I followed with each immunedecient patient, beginning at the crown of the head and working down to the tips of the toes. Brad’s hair was matted with sweat,
and his face was ashen. I peered into his eyes, ears, nose, and throat, and found only some small ulcers on his inner cheeks and
under his tongue—side effects of his treatment. His lungs were clear, and his heart sounds were strong. His abdomen was soft, and
there was no tenderness over his bladder.
“Enough for today,” I said. Brad looked exhausted; it seemed wise to let him rest.
ater that day, I was in the hematology lab, looking at blood cells from a patient with leukemia, when my beeper went off.
“Brad Miller has no blood pressure,” the resident told me when I returned the call. “His temperature is up to a hundred and
four, and we’re moving him to the I.C.U.”
Brad was in septic shock. When bacteria spread through the bloodstream, they can damage the circulation. Septic shock can be
fatal even in people who are otherwise healthy; patients with impaired immunity, like Brad, whose white-blood-cell count had
fallen because of chemotherapy, are at particular risk of dying.
“Do we have a source of infection?” I asked.
“He has what looks like an abscess on his left buttock,” the resident said.
8/6/2020 How Doctors Think | The New Yorker
https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2007/01/29/whats-the-trouble?utm_source=nl&utm_brand=tny&utm_mailing=TNY_Classics_Daily_080520… 6/7
M
Patients who lack enough white blood cells to ght bacteria are prone to infections at sites that are routinely soiled, like the area
between the buttocks. The abscess must have been there when I examined Brad. But I had failed to ask him to roll over so that I
could inspect his buttocks and rectal area.
The resident told me that he had repeated Brad’s cultures and started him on broad-spectrum antibiotics, and that the I.C.U. team
was about to take over.
I was furious with myself. Because I liked Brad, I hadn’t wanted to add to his discomfort and had cut the examination short.
Perhaps I hoped unconsciously that the cause of his fever was trivial and that I would not nd evidence of an infection on his body.
This tendency to make decisions based on what we wish were true is what Croskerry calls an “affective error.” In medicine, this
type of error can have potentially fatal consequences. In the case of Evan McKinley, for example, Pat Croskerry chose to rely on
the ranger’s initial test results—the normal EKG, chest X-ray, and blood tests—all of which suggested a benign diagnosis. He
didn’t arrange for follow-up testing that might have revealed the source of the ranger’s chest pain. Croskerry, who had been an
Olympic rower in his thirties, told me that McKinley had reminded him of himself as an athlete; he believed that this association
contributed to his misdiagnosis.
As soon as I nished my work in the lab, I rushed to the I.C.U. to check on Brad. He was on a respirator and opened his eyes wide
to signal hello. Through an intravenous line attached to one arm, he was receiving pressors, drugs that cause the heart to pump
more effectively and increase the tone of the vessels to help maintain blood pressure. Brad’s heart was holding up, despite all the
Adriamycin he had taken. His platelet count had fallen, as often happens with septic shock, and he was receiving platelet
transfusions. The senior doctor in the I.C.U. had told Brad’s parents, who lived nearby, that he was extremely ill. I saw his parents
sitting in a room next to the I.C.U., their heads bowed. They had not seen me, and I was tempted to avoid them. But I forced
myself to speak to them and offered a few words of encouragement. They thanked me for my care of their son, which only made
me feel worse.
The next morning, I arrived before the residents to review my patients’ charts. Rounds lasted an hour longer than usual, as I
insisted on double-checking each bit of information that the residents offered about the patients in our care.
Brad Miller survived. Slowly, his white-blood-cell count increased, and the infection was resolved. After he left the I.C.U., I told
him that I should have examined him more thoroughly that morning, but I did not explain why I had not. A scan showed that
his sarcoma had shrunk enough for him to undergo surgery without amputation, but a large portion of his thigh muscle had to be
removed along with the tumor. After he recovered, he was no longer able to run, but occasionally I saw him riding his bicycle on
campus.
edical education has not changed substantially since Pat Croskerry and I were trained. Students are still expected to
assimilate large amounts of basic science and apply that knowledge as they are taught practical aspects of patient care.
And young physicians still learn largely by observing more senior members of their eld. (“See one, do one, teach one” remains a
guiding maxim at medical schools.) This approach produces condent and able physicians. Yet the ideal it implies, of the doctor as
a dispassionate and rational actor, is misguided. As Tversky and Kahneman and other cognitive psychologists have shown, when
people are confronted with uncertainty—the situation of every doctor attempting to diagnose a patient—they are susceptible to
unconscious emotions and personal biases, and are more likely to make cognitive errors. Croskerry believes that the rst step
toward incorporating an awareness of heuristics and their liabilities into medical practice is to recognize that how doctors think
can affect their success as much as how much they know, or how much experience they have. “Currently, in medical training, we
fail to recognize the importance of critical thinking and critical reasoning,” Croskerry told me. “The implicit assumption in
medicine is that we know how to think. But we don’t.” ♦
8/6/2020 How Doctors Think | The New Yorker
https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2007/01/29/whats-the-trouble?utm_source=nl&utm_brand=tny&utm_mailing=TNY_Classics_Daily_080520… 7/7
Published in the print edition of the January 29, 2007, issue.
Jerome Groopman, a staff writer at The New Yorker since 1998, writes primarily about medicine and biology.
His latest book is “Your Medical Mind: How to Decide What Is Right for You,” with Dr. Pamela Hartzband.
No comments:
Post a Comment