////////////////////////FSTIVAL TRROR
/////////////////////Today is Oct 12, 2007.Hope for the best, prepare for the worst.~Unknown~
/////////////////Some like it hot
How boomers' failing taste buds are shaping the future of American food
By Sacha Pfeiffer October 7, 2007
ANYONE WHO HAS browsed a supermarket in the last few years can't help but notice the shelves are practically bursting into flames. Spicy Guacamole Pringles. Tyson Hot 'n' Spicy Buffalo Style Chicken Chunks. Mo Hotta Mo Betta Cayenne Garlic Hot Sauce.
Article Tools
Printer friendly
Single page
E-mail to a friend
Ideas RSS feed
Available RSS feeds
Most e-mailed
Reprints & Licensing
Share on Digg
Share on Facebook
Save this article
powered by Del.icio.us
More:
Ideas section
Globe front page
Boston.com
Sign up for:
Globe Headlines e-mail
Breaking News Alerts
Restaurants are no different. McDonald's has its Chipotle BBQ Snack Wrap; Friday's has its Wicked Wings. The spice-driven cooking of India, Thailand, and Sichuan China is responsible for a growing percentage of American takeout dollars every year. It's clear that Americans have developed an addiction to food with sinus-clearing pizzazz.
Why is hot so hot? The conventional explanation is that the nation has an increasingly adventurous palate. Immigration and prosperity have made Americans more sophisticated eaters, pushing wasabi peas into the mainstream, along with chili-Thai lime cashews, cayenne chocolate bars, and other high-octane combinations.
But some food scientists and market researchers think there is a more surprising reason for the broad nationwide shift toward bolder flavors: The baby boomers, that huge, youth-chasing, all-important demographic, are getting old. As they age, they are losing their ability to taste - and turning to spicier, higher-flavor foods to overcome their dulled senses.
Chiefly because of degenerating olfactory nerves, most aging people experience a diminished sense of taste, whether they realize it or not. But unlike previous generations, the nation's 80 million boomers have broad appetites, a full set of teeth, and the spending power to shape the entire food market.
How boomers' failing taste buds are shaping the future of American food
By Sacha Pfeiffer October 7, 2007
ANYONE WHO HAS browsed a supermarket in the last few years can't help but notice the shelves are practically bursting into flames. Spicy Guacamole Pringles. Tyson Hot 'n' Spicy Buffalo Style Chicken Chunks. Mo Hotta Mo Betta Cayenne Garlic Hot Sauce.
Article Tools
Printer friendly
Single page
E-mail to a friend
Ideas RSS feed
Available RSS feeds
Most e-mailed
Reprints & Licensing
Share on Digg
Share on Facebook
Save this article
powered by Del.icio.us
More:
Ideas section
Globe front page
Boston.com
Sign up for:
Globe Headlines e-mail
Breaking News Alerts
Restaurants are no different. McDonald's has its Chipotle BBQ Snack Wrap; Friday's has its Wicked Wings. The spice-driven cooking of India, Thailand, and Sichuan China is responsible for a growing percentage of American takeout dollars every year. It's clear that Americans have developed an addiction to food with sinus-clearing pizzazz.
Why is hot so hot? The conventional explanation is that the nation has an increasingly adventurous palate. Immigration and prosperity have made Americans more sophisticated eaters, pushing wasabi peas into the mainstream, along with chili-Thai lime cashews, cayenne chocolate bars, and other high-octane combinations.
But some food scientists and market researchers think there is a more surprising reason for the broad nationwide shift toward bolder flavors: The baby boomers, that huge, youth-chasing, all-important demographic, are getting old. As they age, they are losing their ability to taste - and turning to spicier, higher-flavor foods to overcome their dulled senses.
Chiefly because of degenerating olfactory nerves, most aging people experience a diminished sense of taste, whether they realize it or not. But unlike previous generations, the nation's 80 million boomers have broad appetites, a full set of teeth, and the spending power to shape the entire food market.
///////////////////Review - The Lucifer EffectUnderstanding How Good People Turn Evilby Philip ZimbardoRandom House, 2007Review by J. Jeremy Wisnewski, Ph.D.Oct 9th 2007 (Volume 11, Issue 41)
Philip Zimbardo, unlike so many people in the world, has a fame that he deserves. He is the mind behind the famous Stanford Prison Experiment--a prison simulation that provides a good deal of evidence for the view that action depends more on situational variables than it does on individual dispositions. In this fascinating experiment, we learn that the individual characteristics of a person will not help us much in predicting their actions. You might think that a self-described 'pacifist' would never think of force-feeding someone. You might think that someone who is intelligent, and generally perceived to be a nice guy, would not engage in systematic acts of degradation and cruelty. On both counts (and many more), as the Stanford Prison Experiment famously shows, you'd be wrong. What makes nice, normal, and intelligent young adults become depressed, violent, self-loathing, or cruel? All it takes is a simulated prison, where everyone knows they are merely acting, and a little over a day.
This, in a nutshell, is the Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE)--and it is the starting point of Zimbardo's new book, The Lucifer Effect: Understanding How Good People Turn Evil. The book begins as a memoir of the experiment, and leads us through a compelling account of the fragility of character in the face of situations. What happens when you randomly assign 19 normal college students to roles as guards and prisoners? We all know the answer: our brightest angels fall from grace. Lucifer is born of the heavens.
Zimbardo presents the riveting tale of this famous experiment--one that took place over 35 years ago, and which has had an immense influence both inside and outside the academy. The Lucifer Effect begins by spelling out, in wonderful and gripping detail, the 6 days of the Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE). This is reader-friendly material of the highest order. Zimbardo writes in the first-person present as he recounts the excitement, surprise, and horror that was created by a simulated prison in the basement of an academic building on the Stanford University campus. Zimbardo is careful to bring out the deep drama and tensions of those days in 1971, ensuring that the reader is surprised by what happens--by the deindividuation and dehumanization that seems to overpower those involved (I have long been familiar with this study, and I couldn't help but find myself in awe at the details. This alone attests to Zimbardo's powerful story-telling abilities). In reading this material, we see full well why Zimbardo is as known as he is: he deserves it. The book is beautifully written, fully engaging, and accessible to anyone who wants a better insight into the SPE and its implications for our understanding of the human condition.
If Zimbardo's book stopped here, simply providing the detailed descriptions of the SPE so vividly, The Lucifer Effect would certainly deserve a place on your shelf. But Zimbardo does not stop here. In the remainder of the book, Zimbardo shows what the significance of the SPE is, and how this can help us understand some of the more deplorable events of our times. In particular, the events that have recently transpired at Abu Ghraib, Guantanamo Bay, Bagram Air Force base, and other sites of detention and abuse at the hands of agents of the US.
The great service of the latter half of Zimbardo's book, however, is not that it will effectively elucidate the relevance of social psychology to Abu Ghraib to those who are already familiar with the research. The book will not accomplish this. It will, however, enable persons unfamiliar with Milgram, the SPE, and other well-known research to come to possess a much greater understanding of how US soldiers might find themselves doing horrible things--indeed, how anyone might find themselves doing horrible things.
Zimbardo's role in the trial of one of the soldiers at Abu Ghraib (Ivan "Chip" Frederick, II), and his access to data about the abuse (torture) and the situational and systemic factors surrounding it, however, put him in a unique position to show his readers what is outrageous about the "bad apples" view of detainee abuse that is the official story of the Bush Administration. For those familiar with the documents surrounding the Abu Ghraib incident, this will come as no surprise (these documents are widely available, most notably and extensively in Greenberg and Dratel, The Torture Papers: The Road to Abu Ghraib, but also elsewhere). For those who have read the memos and the reports, there seems to be little doubt that detainee abuse (torture) was (at least tacitly) condoned, if not outright encouraged, by the administration. But Zimbardo's ability to present this information from his position of social psychological expertise makes the case all the more compelling. Succinctly and plausibly, Zimbardo brings together two widely known phenomena (his earlier SPE work, along with the involvement of the administration in the Abu Ghraib scandal) in a prosecutorial style that will make the best lawyers proud. Going beyond the accusatory reports on Abu Ghraib, Zimbardo convincingly argues that the military system itself created the conditions in which unsupervised cruelty could flourish. Far from offering an excuse for those soldiers on the ground level who had a direct hand in torture, Zimbardo emphasizes the responsibility of those who created the situation at Abu Ghraib itself, as well as for those in the administration who created larger systemic conditions that made situationally-induced torture all the more likely. (Unlike many other indictments, Zimbardo's includes President George W. Bush and Vice President Dick Cheney). This is not a way of excusing torturers. It is a way of insuring that all responsible are made to be so.
Despite the vast amount of information in the book that encourages pessimism about the human animal--information that will undoubtedly worry the reader about what he or she is capable of--Zimbardo ends with a chapter that simply refuses to permit us our pessimism. If we are the creatures of situations and systems, we are not the slaves of these things. The banality of evil--the very thing so illuminatingly revealed by the SPE and by Abu Ghraib--is justified by the same arguments that lead us to justify the view of heroism as an equally banal phenomenon.
most people who become perpetrators of evil deeds are directly comparable to those who become perpetrators of heroic deeds, alike in being just ordinary, average people. Neither attribute is the direct consequence of unique dispositional tendencies...any of us could as easily become heroes as perpetrators of evil depending on how we are influenced by situational forces. The imperative becomes discovering how to limit, constrain, and prevent the situational and systemic forces that propel some of us to social pathology. (485-486).
To this end, Zimbardo offers some considerations regarding how we, as individuals immersed in incredibly complex situations, can attempt to navigate through them without becoming lost in anonymous roles, swept up into a de-individuated present that makes us strangers even to ourselves.
This, I would like to point out, is one way in which Zimbardo himself borders on the heroic. He is an ordinary human being that has found himself in a rather particular circumstance: that of an innovative social scientist--and one that has seen the darker sides of human nature. Always cognizant of the evil that emerged even in himself as he allowed the SPE to continue past the point at which it should have been terminated, Zimbardo recognizes that work with human subjects must be repaid with high dividends--dividends that are not guaranteed by the research results alone. One must be an active citizen, on Zimbardo's view, to insure that the pain and cruelty created by one's work is made up for in spades. Zimbardo has spent a good deal of his life doing just this, and The Lucifer Effect ought to be read as an ongoing part of this endeavor. It teaches us that we are weaker than we think, but strong enough to try to make a difference.
© 2007 J. Jeremy Wisnewski
J. Jeremy Wisnewski, PhD, Department of Philosophy, Hartwick College, Oneonta, NY, 13820, USA
Philip Zimbardo, unlike so many people in the world, has a fame that he deserves. He is the mind behind the famous Stanford Prison Experiment--a prison simulation that provides a good deal of evidence for the view that action depends more on situational variables than it does on individual dispositions. In this fascinating experiment, we learn that the individual characteristics of a person will not help us much in predicting their actions. You might think that a self-described 'pacifist' would never think of force-feeding someone. You might think that someone who is intelligent, and generally perceived to be a nice guy, would not engage in systematic acts of degradation and cruelty. On both counts (and many more), as the Stanford Prison Experiment famously shows, you'd be wrong. What makes nice, normal, and intelligent young adults become depressed, violent, self-loathing, or cruel? All it takes is a simulated prison, where everyone knows they are merely acting, and a little over a day.
This, in a nutshell, is the Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE)--and it is the starting point of Zimbardo's new book, The Lucifer Effect: Understanding How Good People Turn Evil. The book begins as a memoir of the experiment, and leads us through a compelling account of the fragility of character in the face of situations. What happens when you randomly assign 19 normal college students to roles as guards and prisoners? We all know the answer: our brightest angels fall from grace. Lucifer is born of the heavens.
Zimbardo presents the riveting tale of this famous experiment--one that took place over 35 years ago, and which has had an immense influence both inside and outside the academy. The Lucifer Effect begins by spelling out, in wonderful and gripping detail, the 6 days of the Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE). This is reader-friendly material of the highest order. Zimbardo writes in the first-person present as he recounts the excitement, surprise, and horror that was created by a simulated prison in the basement of an academic building on the Stanford University campus. Zimbardo is careful to bring out the deep drama and tensions of those days in 1971, ensuring that the reader is surprised by what happens--by the deindividuation and dehumanization that seems to overpower those involved (I have long been familiar with this study, and I couldn't help but find myself in awe at the details. This alone attests to Zimbardo's powerful story-telling abilities). In reading this material, we see full well why Zimbardo is as known as he is: he deserves it. The book is beautifully written, fully engaging, and accessible to anyone who wants a better insight into the SPE and its implications for our understanding of the human condition.
If Zimbardo's book stopped here, simply providing the detailed descriptions of the SPE so vividly, The Lucifer Effect would certainly deserve a place on your shelf. But Zimbardo does not stop here. In the remainder of the book, Zimbardo shows what the significance of the SPE is, and how this can help us understand some of the more deplorable events of our times. In particular, the events that have recently transpired at Abu Ghraib, Guantanamo Bay, Bagram Air Force base, and other sites of detention and abuse at the hands of agents of the US.
The great service of the latter half of Zimbardo's book, however, is not that it will effectively elucidate the relevance of social psychology to Abu Ghraib to those who are already familiar with the research. The book will not accomplish this. It will, however, enable persons unfamiliar with Milgram, the SPE, and other well-known research to come to possess a much greater understanding of how US soldiers might find themselves doing horrible things--indeed, how anyone might find themselves doing horrible things.
Zimbardo's role in the trial of one of the soldiers at Abu Ghraib (Ivan "Chip" Frederick, II), and his access to data about the abuse (torture) and the situational and systemic factors surrounding it, however, put him in a unique position to show his readers what is outrageous about the "bad apples" view of detainee abuse that is the official story of the Bush Administration. For those familiar with the documents surrounding the Abu Ghraib incident, this will come as no surprise (these documents are widely available, most notably and extensively in Greenberg and Dratel, The Torture Papers: The Road to Abu Ghraib, but also elsewhere). For those who have read the memos and the reports, there seems to be little doubt that detainee abuse (torture) was (at least tacitly) condoned, if not outright encouraged, by the administration. But Zimbardo's ability to present this information from his position of social psychological expertise makes the case all the more compelling. Succinctly and plausibly, Zimbardo brings together two widely known phenomena (his earlier SPE work, along with the involvement of the administration in the Abu Ghraib scandal) in a prosecutorial style that will make the best lawyers proud. Going beyond the accusatory reports on Abu Ghraib, Zimbardo convincingly argues that the military system itself created the conditions in which unsupervised cruelty could flourish. Far from offering an excuse for those soldiers on the ground level who had a direct hand in torture, Zimbardo emphasizes the responsibility of those who created the situation at Abu Ghraib itself, as well as for those in the administration who created larger systemic conditions that made situationally-induced torture all the more likely. (Unlike many other indictments, Zimbardo's includes President George W. Bush and Vice President Dick Cheney). This is not a way of excusing torturers. It is a way of insuring that all responsible are made to be so.
Despite the vast amount of information in the book that encourages pessimism about the human animal--information that will undoubtedly worry the reader about what he or she is capable of--Zimbardo ends with a chapter that simply refuses to permit us our pessimism. If we are the creatures of situations and systems, we are not the slaves of these things. The banality of evil--the very thing so illuminatingly revealed by the SPE and by Abu Ghraib--is justified by the same arguments that lead us to justify the view of heroism as an equally banal phenomenon.
most people who become perpetrators of evil deeds are directly comparable to those who become perpetrators of heroic deeds, alike in being just ordinary, average people. Neither attribute is the direct consequence of unique dispositional tendencies...any of us could as easily become heroes as perpetrators of evil depending on how we are influenced by situational forces. The imperative becomes discovering how to limit, constrain, and prevent the situational and systemic forces that propel some of us to social pathology. (485-486).
To this end, Zimbardo offers some considerations regarding how we, as individuals immersed in incredibly complex situations, can attempt to navigate through them without becoming lost in anonymous roles, swept up into a de-individuated present that makes us strangers even to ourselves.
This, I would like to point out, is one way in which Zimbardo himself borders on the heroic. He is an ordinary human being that has found himself in a rather particular circumstance: that of an innovative social scientist--and one that has seen the darker sides of human nature. Always cognizant of the evil that emerged even in himself as he allowed the SPE to continue past the point at which it should have been terminated, Zimbardo recognizes that work with human subjects must be repaid with high dividends--dividends that are not guaranteed by the research results alone. One must be an active citizen, on Zimbardo's view, to insure that the pain and cruelty created by one's work is made up for in spades. Zimbardo has spent a good deal of his life doing just this, and The Lucifer Effect ought to be read as an ongoing part of this endeavor. It teaches us that we are weaker than we think, but strong enough to try to make a difference.
© 2007 J. Jeremy Wisnewski
J. Jeremy Wisnewski, PhD, Department of Philosophy, Hartwick College, Oneonta, NY, 13820, USA
///////////////////////Other universes may be detectable, published study claims
Oct. 11, 2007Special to World Science
If there are other universes out there—as some scientists propose—then one or more of them might be detectable, a new study suggests.Such a finding, “while currently speculative even in principle, and probably far-off in practice, would surely constitute an epochal discovery,” researchers wrote in a paper detailing their study. The work appears in the September issue of the research journal Physical Review D.Cosmologists generally hold that even if other universes exist, a controversial idea itself, they wouldn’t be visible, and that testing for their existence would be hard at best.
A half-sky map of slight temperature variations in the cosmic microwave background radiation, thought to map structures in the very early universe. Blue stands for colder areas; red for hotter regions, where it's believed matter was denser. These dense regions are thought to have later become galaxy-rich zones. The boxed area marks an unusual "cold spot" astronomers recognize in the data. An unexplained giant cosmic void has also been found in the direction of that spot. In a new study, theoretical physicists argue that some sort of irregularity in the microwave background, and in matter distribution, might indicate where our universe once knocked into another one. But the researchers take no position on whether this cold spot could be the anomaly they're looking for. Much more work is needed, they say. (Image courtesy WMAP Science Team, NASA)
But the new study, by three scientists at the University of California, Santa Cruz, proposes that neighboring universes might leave a visible mark on our own—if, perchance, they have knocked into it. For such a scar to be detectable, they add, the collision might have had to take place when our universe was very young. Just how the bruise might look remains to be clarified, they say.“The question of what the aftermath of a collision might be is still quite open,” wrote Matthew C. Johnson, one of the researchers, in an email. One theory even holds that a clash between universes could destroy the cosmos we know. But Johnson, now at the California Institute of Technology in Pasadena, Calif., and colleagues are examining quite a different sort of scenario.Several lines of reasoning in modern physics have led to proposals that there are other universes. It’s a rather dodgy concept on its face, because strictly speaking, “the universe” means everything that exists. But in practice, cosmologists often loosen the definition and just speak of “a universe” as some sort of self-enclosed whole with its own physical laws.Such a picture, in concept, allows for other universes with different laws. These realms are often called “bubble universes” or “pocket universes”—partly to sidestep the awkward definitional issue, and partly because many theorists do indeed portray them as bubble-like.A key thread of reasoning behind the idea of bubble universes, which are sometimes collectively called a “multiverse,” is the finding that seemingly empty space contains energy, known as vacuum energy. Some theorize that under certain circumstances this energy can be converted into an explosively growing, new universe—the same process believed to have given rise to ours. Theoretical physicists including Michio Kaku of city College of New York argue that this might go on constantly—he has called it a “continual genesis”—creating many universes, coexisting not unlike bubbles in a foamy bath.How might one detect another universe? Johnson and his colleagues reason that any collision between bubbles would, like all collisions, produce aftereffects that propagate into both chambers. These effects would probably take the form of some material ejected into both sides, Johnson said, although just what is unknown. This would in turn affect the distribution of matter in each pocket universe.If such collisions happened recently, they might be undetectable because our universe might be too huge to be markedly affected; but not so if the events took place long enough ago, according to the University of California team, whose paper is also posted online. If a knock occurred when our expanding universe was still very small, they argue, then the aftermath might still be visible, blown up in size along with everything else since then.When the universe was less than a thousandth its present size, it’s thought to have undergone a transformation. As it expanded, it became cool enough for atoms to form. It then also became transparent. Before that, everything had been a thick fog, but with tiny variations in its density at different points; denser parts would eventually grow and coalesce into galaxies.This fog is still visible, because many of the light waves it gave off are just now reaching us: this is how astronomers explain a faint glow that permeates space, called the cosmic microwave background. It represents the edge of our visible universe and is detected in all directions of the sky.A collision would lead to a rearranged pattern of density fluctuations in this background, according to the University of California team. It’s unclear just how this rearrangement would look, but it would probably appear as some sort of area of irregularity centered on a patch of the sky—since “each collision will affect a disc on our sky,” Johnson wrote in an email. An analogy: if you lived in a beach ball and it bounced off another beach ball, you’d see a change in a circular area of your wall.“Nothing like this has presently been observed, although no one has ever looked for this particular signal,” Johnson added. On the other hand, researchers have found at least one striking irregularity in the background glow—a “cold spot,” thought to be related to a vast and anomalous void in the cosmos. Could that be the mark of a separate universe? “I’m going to remain completely noncommittal” on that, Johnson said. “I can’t even tell you if it would be a hot spot or a cold spot.” Temperature variations in the cosmic microwave background are believed to reflect density variations in the early universe.Johnson and colleagues stressed that their proposal may be only the beginning of a long, painstaking research program. “Connecting this prediction to real observational signatures will entail both difficult and comprehensive future work (and probably no small measure of good luck),” they wrote. But “it appears worth pursuing.”
Oct. 11, 2007Special to World Science
If there are other universes out there—as some scientists propose—then one or more of them might be detectable, a new study suggests.Such a finding, “while currently speculative even in principle, and probably far-off in practice, would surely constitute an epochal discovery,” researchers wrote in a paper detailing their study. The work appears in the September issue of the research journal Physical Review D.Cosmologists generally hold that even if other universes exist, a controversial idea itself, they wouldn’t be visible, and that testing for their existence would be hard at best.
A half-sky map of slight temperature variations in the cosmic microwave background radiation, thought to map structures in the very early universe. Blue stands for colder areas; red for hotter regions, where it's believed matter was denser. These dense regions are thought to have later become galaxy-rich zones. The boxed area marks an unusual "cold spot" astronomers recognize in the data. An unexplained giant cosmic void has also been found in the direction of that spot. In a new study, theoretical physicists argue that some sort of irregularity in the microwave background, and in matter distribution, might indicate where our universe once knocked into another one. But the researchers take no position on whether this cold spot could be the anomaly they're looking for. Much more work is needed, they say. (Image courtesy WMAP Science Team, NASA)
But the new study, by three scientists at the University of California, Santa Cruz, proposes that neighboring universes might leave a visible mark on our own—if, perchance, they have knocked into it. For such a scar to be detectable, they add, the collision might have had to take place when our universe was very young. Just how the bruise might look remains to be clarified, they say.“The question of what the aftermath of a collision might be is still quite open,” wrote Matthew C. Johnson, one of the researchers, in an email. One theory even holds that a clash between universes could destroy the cosmos we know. But Johnson, now at the California Institute of Technology in Pasadena, Calif., and colleagues are examining quite a different sort of scenario.Several lines of reasoning in modern physics have led to proposals that there are other universes. It’s a rather dodgy concept on its face, because strictly speaking, “the universe” means everything that exists. But in practice, cosmologists often loosen the definition and just speak of “a universe” as some sort of self-enclosed whole with its own physical laws.Such a picture, in concept, allows for other universes with different laws. These realms are often called “bubble universes” or “pocket universes”—partly to sidestep the awkward definitional issue, and partly because many theorists do indeed portray them as bubble-like.A key thread of reasoning behind the idea of bubble universes, which are sometimes collectively called a “multiverse,” is the finding that seemingly empty space contains energy, known as vacuum energy. Some theorize that under certain circumstances this energy can be converted into an explosively growing, new universe—the same process believed to have given rise to ours. Theoretical physicists including Michio Kaku of city College of New York argue that this might go on constantly—he has called it a “continual genesis”—creating many universes, coexisting not unlike bubbles in a foamy bath.How might one detect another universe? Johnson and his colleagues reason that any collision between bubbles would, like all collisions, produce aftereffects that propagate into both chambers. These effects would probably take the form of some material ejected into both sides, Johnson said, although just what is unknown. This would in turn affect the distribution of matter in each pocket universe.If such collisions happened recently, they might be undetectable because our universe might be too huge to be markedly affected; but not so if the events took place long enough ago, according to the University of California team, whose paper is also posted online. If a knock occurred when our expanding universe was still very small, they argue, then the aftermath might still be visible, blown up in size along with everything else since then.When the universe was less than a thousandth its present size, it’s thought to have undergone a transformation. As it expanded, it became cool enough for atoms to form. It then also became transparent. Before that, everything had been a thick fog, but with tiny variations in its density at different points; denser parts would eventually grow and coalesce into galaxies.This fog is still visible, because many of the light waves it gave off are just now reaching us: this is how astronomers explain a faint glow that permeates space, called the cosmic microwave background. It represents the edge of our visible universe and is detected in all directions of the sky.A collision would lead to a rearranged pattern of density fluctuations in this background, according to the University of California team. It’s unclear just how this rearrangement would look, but it would probably appear as some sort of area of irregularity centered on a patch of the sky—since “each collision will affect a disc on our sky,” Johnson wrote in an email. An analogy: if you lived in a beach ball and it bounced off another beach ball, you’d see a change in a circular area of your wall.“Nothing like this has presently been observed, although no one has ever looked for this particular signal,” Johnson added. On the other hand, researchers have found at least one striking irregularity in the background glow—a “cold spot,” thought to be related to a vast and anomalous void in the cosmos. Could that be the mark of a separate universe? “I’m going to remain completely noncommittal” on that, Johnson said. “I can’t even tell you if it would be a hot spot or a cold spot.” Temperature variations in the cosmic microwave background are believed to reflect density variations in the early universe.Johnson and colleagues stressed that their proposal may be only the beginning of a long, painstaking research program. “Connecting this prediction to real observational signatures will entail both difficult and comprehensive future work (and probably no small measure of good luck),” they wrote. But “it appears worth pursuing.”
////////////////////ANTR CINGULATE GYRUS-CENTRE OF SELF-CONTROL
When our vices get the better of usWhat happens in the brain when we just can’t say no?
Oct. 11, 2007Courtesy Association for Psychological Scienceand World Science staff
Drug abuse, crime and obesity are but a few of the problems modern society faces, but they all have one thing in common: people’s failure to control themselves in the face of temptation. While the ability to restrain our impulses is a defining feature of the human animal, its failure is one of society’s central problems. So why do we so often lack this crucial ability?
The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), marked in red. This brain area is crucial to self-control, researchers say.
As humans, we have limited resources to control ourselves, researchers say; all acts of control draw from one source. So when using this resource in one domain, such as dieting, we’re more likely to run out of it in another domain, like studying hard. Once these resources run out, our self-control ability is diminished, according to scientists. The dieter is more likely to eat chocolate, the student to watch TV, and the politician to accept a bribe.In a recent study, Michael Inzlicht of the University of Toronto Scarborough and colleague Jennifer N. Gutsell offer an account of what’s happening in the brain when our vices get the better of us.Inzlicht and Gutsell asked participants to suppress their emotions while watching an upsetting movie. The idea was to deplete their resources for self-control. The participants reported their ability to suppress their feelings on a scale from one to nine. Then, they completed a Stroop task, which involves naming the color of printed words (i.e. saying red when reading the word “green” written in red), yet another task that requires self-control.The researchers found that those who suppressed their emotions performed worse on the task, indicating that they had used up their self-control resources while holding back their tears during the film. An electroencephalogram (EEG), a recording of electrical activity in the brain, confirmed the results, they said. Normally, when a person deviates from their goals (in this case, wanting to read the word, not the color of the font), increased activity occurs in a part of the brain called the anterior cingulate cortex, which alerts the person that they are off-track. The researchers found weaker activity in this brain region during the Stroop task in those who had suppressed their feelings. In other words, after engaging in one act of self-control this brain system seems to fail during the next act, they said. The findings, which appear in the November issue of the research journal Psychological Science, have implications for future interventions aiming to help people change their behavior, the researchers argued. Most notably, they said, the results suggest that if people even temporarily don’t realize they have lost control, they will be unable to stop or change their behavior on their own.
Oct. 11, 2007Courtesy Association for Psychological Scienceand World Science staff
Drug abuse, crime and obesity are but a few of the problems modern society faces, but they all have one thing in common: people’s failure to control themselves in the face of temptation. While the ability to restrain our impulses is a defining feature of the human animal, its failure is one of society’s central problems. So why do we so often lack this crucial ability?
The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), marked in red. This brain area is crucial to self-control, researchers say.
As humans, we have limited resources to control ourselves, researchers say; all acts of control draw from one source. So when using this resource in one domain, such as dieting, we’re more likely to run out of it in another domain, like studying hard. Once these resources run out, our self-control ability is diminished, according to scientists. The dieter is more likely to eat chocolate, the student to watch TV, and the politician to accept a bribe.In a recent study, Michael Inzlicht of the University of Toronto Scarborough and colleague Jennifer N. Gutsell offer an account of what’s happening in the brain when our vices get the better of us.Inzlicht and Gutsell asked participants to suppress their emotions while watching an upsetting movie. The idea was to deplete their resources for self-control. The participants reported their ability to suppress their feelings on a scale from one to nine. Then, they completed a Stroop task, which involves naming the color of printed words (i.e. saying red when reading the word “green” written in red), yet another task that requires self-control.The researchers found that those who suppressed their emotions performed worse on the task, indicating that they had used up their self-control resources while holding back their tears during the film. An electroencephalogram (EEG), a recording of electrical activity in the brain, confirmed the results, they said. Normally, when a person deviates from their goals (in this case, wanting to read the word, not the color of the font), increased activity occurs in a part of the brain called the anterior cingulate cortex, which alerts the person that they are off-track. The researchers found weaker activity in this brain region during the Stroop task in those who had suppressed their feelings. In other words, after engaging in one act of self-control this brain system seems to fail during the next act, they said. The findings, which appear in the November issue of the research journal Psychological Science, have implications for future interventions aiming to help people change their behavior, the researchers argued. Most notably, they said, the results suggest that if people even temporarily don’t realize they have lost control, they will be unable to stop or change their behavior on their own.
No comments:
Post a Comment